bioacoustics-l
[Top] [All Lists]

Bioacoustic Articles in Behaviour 145 - No 2 & 3 (February & March)

To: "Bioacoustic-L" <>
Subject: Bioacoustic Articles in Behaviour 145 - No 2 & 3 (February & March)
From: "Frank Veit" <>
Date: Fri, 25 Jan 2008 16:01:53 +0100
(Abstracts see below)

Behaviour vol. 145 no. 2 & 3 (February & March 2008)
http://www.ingentaconnect.com/content/brill/beh/2008/00000145/00000002
http://www.ingentaconnect.com/content/brill/beh/2008/00000145/00000003

Sanvito, S, F Galimberti & EH Miller (2008) Development of aggressive vocalizations in male southern elephant seals (Mirounga leonina): maturation or learning? Behaviour 145: 137-170.

Neudorf, DLH, BJM Stutchbury & WH Piper (2008) The function of breeding season chip calls by female hooded warblers (Wilsonia citrina). Behaviour 145: 231-250.

Leighty, KA, J Soltis, K Leong & A Savage (2008) Antiphonal exchanges in African elephants (Loxodonta africana): collective response to a shared stimulus, social facilitation, or true communicative event? Behaviour 145: 297-312.

Anderson, RC, WA Searcy & S Nowicki (2008) Testing the function of song-matching in birds: responses of eastern male song sparrows Melospiza melodia to partial song-matching. Behaviour 145: 347-363.



- Abstracts -

Sanvito, S, F Galimberti & EH Miller (2008) Development of aggressive vocalizations in male southern elephant seals (Mirounga leonina): maturation or learning? Behaviour 145: 137-170. Vocalizations are an important component of male elephant seal agonistic behaviour. Acoustic and behavioural components of vocalizations emitted during agonistic contests show gross differences between young and old males, but the variation with age depends on the specific feature. Vocalizations become more frequent and effective at later ages. Acoustic features that are constrained by structural phenotype, which changes with age, also should change with age, while acoustic features that are independent from structural phenotype should show no relationship with age. We demonstrate that, in southern elephant seals, formant frequencies, which are constrained by the vocal tract length and, therefore, by body size, show a clear decrease with age, whereas temporal and structural features of sounds, which potentially are unconstrained, show no correlation with age. Formants ontogeny seems, therefore, to be mostly the result of body maturation, and hence formants may be reliable signals of age. In contrast, acoustic features such as temporal features and syllable structure, are free to change, and hence may serve as the raw material for vocal learning and individual recognition.

Neudorf, DLH, BJM Stutchbury & WH Piper (2008) The function of breeding season chip calls by female hooded warblers (Wilsonia citrina). Behaviour 145: 231-250. We document use of 'chip' vocalizations by breeding female hooded warblers (Wilsonia citrina) and propose explanations for the function of the calls. Fertile females (building or laying stage) chip differently than incubating females, via a lower chip rate, shorter bouts of chipping and more bouts per hour. Females responded to playbacks of fertile chipping and a female model on their territories by approaching and chip calling. Resident males were generally not responsive to their mate's chip calls and did not move closer when their mates chipped; however, neighbouring males often intruded onto territories in response to female chip calls. Neighbouring males did not distinguish between playbacks of fertile and incubation chip calls, but responded to both at equally low frequencies. Radio-tracked neighbouring males intruded onto territories significantly more frequently when females were chipping than when they were silent. Such intrusions likely function as extra-pair mating attempts. Finally, variation in call pattern among fertile females did not correlate with frequency of extra-pair young. Our study provides some support that chip calls of hooded warblers function in female territory defence and attraction of potential extra-pair males but further testing of these hypotheses is required.

Leighty, KA, J Soltis, K Leong & A Savage (2008) Antiphonal exchanges in African elephants (Loxodonta africana): collective response to a shared stimulus, social facilitation, or true communicative event? Behaviour 145: 297-312. Female African elephants are thought to exchange 'rumble' vocalizations, but such temporally associated calls may not constitute communicative events. Affiliated females are more likely to engage in antiphonal calling, but affiliation is defined according to time spent in proximity. Affiliated partners may vocalize in sequence simply because their proximity causes them to collectively respond to shared external stimuli or due to a social facilitation effect. We used bi-variate and partial correlation analyses to test for the independent effects of the strength of the social relationship and distance between vocal partners on the likelihood of a vocal response. Female African elephants at Disney's Animal Kingdom were video-taped and outfitted with audio-recording collars that allowed for the individual identification of low-frequency rumbles. Affiliation had a strong influence on response likelihood, even after controlling for the effects of the distance between vocalizing partners. Further, the distance between vocalizing partners did not correlate with response likelihood, and factoring out the effects of affiliation did not significantly alter this result. These results suggest that rumble exchanges are communicative events that reflect social bonds, not simply artifacts of increased proximity and, therefore, provide support for functional hypotheses concerning rumble exchanges in wild African elephants.

Anderson, RC, WA Searcy & S Nowicki (2008) Testing the function of song-matching in birds: responses of eastern male song sparrows Melospiza melodia to partial song-matching. Behaviour 145: 347-363. Song-matching has been hypothesized to be a signal of aggressive intentions whereby matching an opponent signals that the singer is likely to attack. Theory predicts that an aggressive signal should impose a cost that enforces the signal's reliability. A receiver-dependent cost imposed by the matched bird's aggressive retaliation has been proposed for song-matching. We tested for such a cost for partial song-matching in an eastern population of song sparrows where males lack the shared song types necessary for song type matching, but can perform partial song-matching using shared song segments. We tested aggressive response, as measured by average distance to a playback speaker, to partial-matching songs and non-matching songs. We predicted a stronger aggressive response to partial-matching songs, as has been shown for whole song-matching in western song sparrow populations. The birds in our study responded no differently to partial-matching and non-matching songs. Neither the distance to the playback speaker nor singing responses differed between playback treatments. Our results do not support a receiver-dependent cost to partial song-matching, as would be expected if partial-matching is a direct threat. Instead, we suggest that partial song-matching functions as a signal of attention.

<Prev in Thread] Current Thread [Next in Thread>
  • Bioacoustic Articles in Behaviour 145 - No 2 & 3 (February & March), Frank Veit <=
Admin

The University of NSW School of Computer and Engineering takes no responsibility for the contents of this archive. It is purely a compilation of material sent by many people to the Bioacoustics-L mailing list. It has not been checked for accuracy nor its content verified in any way. If you wish to get material removed from the archive or have other queries about the archive e-mail Andrew Taylor at this address: andrewt@cse.unsw.EDU.AU