bioacoustics-l
[Top] [All Lists]

Bioacoustic Articles in Behaviour 145 - No 2 & 3 (February & March)

To: "Bioacoustic-L" <>
Subject: Bioacoustic Articles in Behaviour 145 - No 2 & 3 (February & March)
From: "Frank Veit" <>
Date: Fri, 25 Jan 2008 16:01:53 +0100
(Abstracts see below)

Behaviour vol. 145 no. 2 & 3 (February & March 2008)
http://www.ingentaconnect.com/content/brill/beh/2008/00000145/00000002
http://www.ingentaconnect.com/content/brill/beh/2008/00000145/00000003

Sanvito, S, F Galimberti & EH Miller (2008) Development of aggressive vocalizations in male southern elephant seals (Mirounga leonina): maturation or learning? Behaviour 145: 137-170.
Neudorf, DLH, BJM Stutchbury & WH Piper (2008) The function of breeding 
season chip calls by female hooded warblers (Wilsonia citrina). Behaviour 
145: 231-250.
Leighty, KA, J Soltis, K Leong & A Savage (2008) Antiphonal exchanges in 
African elephants (Loxodonta africana): collective response to a shared 
stimulus, social facilitation, or true communicative event? Behaviour 145: 
297-312.
Anderson, RC, WA Searcy & S Nowicki (2008) Testing the function of 
song-matching in birds: responses of eastern male song sparrows Melospiza 
melodia to partial song-matching. Behaviour 145: 347-363.


- Abstracts -

Sanvito, S, F Galimberti & EH Miller (2008) Development of aggressive vocalizations in male southern elephant seals (Mirounga leonina): maturation or learning? Behaviour 145: 137-170. Vocalizations are an important component of male elephant seal agonistic behaviour. Acoustic and behavioural components of vocalizations emitted during agonistic contests show gross differences between young and old males, but the variation with age depends on the specific feature. Vocalizations become more frequent and effective at later ages. Acoustic features that are constrained by structural phenotype, which changes with age, also should change with age, while acoustic features that are independent from structural phenotype should show no relationship with age. We demonstrate that, in southern elephant seals, formant frequencies, which are constrained by the vocal tract length and, therefore, by body size, show a clear decrease with age, whereas temporal and structural features of sounds, which potentially are unconstrained, show no correlation with age. Formants ontogeny seems, therefore, to be mostly the result of body maturation, and hence formants may be reliable signals of age. In contrast, acoustic features such as temporal features and syllable structure, are free to change, and hence may serve as the raw material for vocal learning and individual recognition.
Neudorf, DLH, BJM Stutchbury & WH Piper (2008) The function of breeding 
season chip calls by female hooded warblers (Wilsonia citrina). Behaviour 
145: 231-250.
We document use of 'chip' vocalizations by breeding female hooded warblers 
(Wilsonia citrina) and propose explanations for the function of the calls. 
Fertile females (building or laying stage) chip differently than incubating 
females, via a lower chip rate, shorter bouts of chipping and more bouts per 
hour. Females responded to playbacks of fertile chipping and a female model 
on their territories by approaching and chip calling. Resident males were 
generally not responsive to their mate's chip calls and did not move closer 
when their mates chipped; however, neighbouring males often intruded onto 
territories in response to female chip calls. Neighbouring males did not 
distinguish between playbacks of fertile and incubation chip calls, but 
responded to both at equally low frequencies. Radio-tracked neighbouring 
males intruded onto territories significantly more frequently when females 
were chipping than when they were silent. Such intrusions likely function as 
extra-pair mating attempts. Finally, variation in call pattern among fertile 
females did not correlate with frequency of extra-pair young. Our study 
provides some support that chip calls of hooded warblers function in female 
territory defence and attraction of potential extra-pair males but further 
testing of these hypotheses is required.
Leighty, KA, J Soltis, K Leong & A Savage (2008) Antiphonal exchanges in 
African elephants (Loxodonta africana): collective response to a shared 
stimulus, social facilitation, or true communicative event? Behaviour 145: 
297-312.
Female African elephants are thought to exchange 'rumble' vocalizations, but 
such temporally associated calls may not constitute communicative events. 
Affiliated females are more likely to engage in antiphonal calling, but 
affiliation is defined according to time spent in proximity. Affiliated 
partners may vocalize in sequence simply because their proximity causes them 
to collectively respond to shared external stimuli or due to a social 
facilitation effect. We used bi-variate and partial correlation analyses to 
test for the independent effects of the strength of the social relationship 
and distance between vocal partners on the likelihood of a vocal response. 
Female African elephants at Disney's Animal Kingdom were video-taped and 
outfitted with audio-recording collars that allowed for the individual 
identification of low-frequency rumbles. Affiliation had a strong influence 
on response likelihood, even after controlling for the effects of the 
distance between vocalizing partners. Further, the distance between 
vocalizing partners did not correlate with response likelihood, and 
factoring out the effects of affiliation did not significantly alter this 
result. These results suggest that rumble exchanges are communicative events 
that reflect social bonds, not simply artifacts of increased proximity and, 
therefore, provide support for functional hypotheses concerning rumble 
exchanges in wild African elephants.
Anderson, RC, WA Searcy & S Nowicki (2008) Testing the function of 
song-matching in birds: responses of eastern male song sparrows Melospiza 
melodia to partial song-matching. Behaviour 145: 347-363.
Song-matching has been hypothesized to be a signal of aggressive intentions 
whereby matching an opponent signals that the singer is likely to attack. 
Theory predicts that an aggressive signal should impose a cost that enforces 
the signal's reliability. A receiver-dependent cost imposed by the matched 
bird's aggressive retaliation has been proposed for song-matching. We tested 
for such a cost for partial song-matching in an eastern population of song 
sparrows where males lack the shared song types necessary for song type 
matching, but can perform partial song-matching using shared song segments. 
We tested aggressive response, as measured by average distance to a playback 
speaker, to partial-matching songs and non-matching songs. We predicted a 
stronger aggressive response to partial-matching songs, as has been shown 
for whole song-matching in western song sparrow populations. The birds in 
our study responded no differently to partial-matching and non-matching 
songs. Neither the distance to the playback speaker nor singing responses 
differed between playback treatments. Our results do not support a 
receiver-dependent cost to partial song-matching, as would be expected if 
partial-matching is a direct threat. Instead, we suggest that partial 
song-matching functions as a signal of attention. 





<Prev in Thread] Current Thread [Next in Thread>
  • Bioacoustic Articles in Behaviour 145 - No 2 & 3 (February & March), Frank Veit <=
Admin

The University of NSW School of Computer and Engineering takes no responsibility for the contents of this archive. It is purely a compilation of material sent by many people to the Bioacoustics-L mailing list. It has not been checked for accuracy nor its content verified in any way. If you wish to get material removed from the archive or have other queries about the archive e-mail Andrew Taylor at this address: andrewt@cse.unsw.EDU.AU