bioacoustics-l
[Top] [All Lists]

Bioacoustic Articles in Behaviour 143, 12 (December 2006)

To: "Bioacoustic-L" <>
Subject: Bioacoustic Articles in Behaviour 143, 12 (December 2006)
From: "Frank Veit" <>
Date: Sun, 14 Jan 2007 11:28:09 +0100 (MET)
Behaviour 143, 12 (December 2006)
http://www.ingentaconnect.com/content/brill/beh/2006/00000143/00000012

Couldridge, VCK & MJ van Staaden (2006) Female preferences for male calling songs in the bladder grasshopper Bullacris membracioides. Behaviour 143: 1439-1456.

Baker, MC & DE Gammon (2006) Persistence and change of vocal signals in natural populations of chickadees: annual sampling of the gargle call over eight seasons. Behaviour 143: 1473-1509.

Poesel, A & T Dabelsteen (2006) Three vocalization types in the blue tit Cyanistes caeruleus: a test of the different signal-value hypothesis. Behaviour 143: 1529-1545.

Nuechterlein, GL & D Buitron (2006) Advertising by western grebes: bout-length variability and signal confusion in a multiple-use call. Behaviour 143: 1547-1562.


Abstracts

Couldridge, VCK & MJ van Staaden (2006) Female preferences for male calling songs in the bladder grasshopper Bullacris membracioides. Behaviour 143: 1439-1456.

Male acoustic signals and the information they convey are often critical determinants of female mate choice. Bladder grasshoppers are one of numerous orthopteran taxa utilizing sound as the basis of courtship and ultimately mating. However, despite the extreme specializations for long-distance acoustic communication in this family, female mating preferences for male calls have not been previously investigated. Here we examine female acoustic responses to playbacks of male calls in Bullacris membracioides. Females were tested in three separate contexts, viz. response to conspecific calls of different individuals, response to degraded conspecific calls, and response to the calls of two heterospecifics. Female response was significantly correlated with seven of eight measured call features within B. membracioides, indicating sexual selection to be operating in this species. Females also responded to conspecific calls with degradation levels equivalent to a male calling 150 m away, but intensity equivalent to one at 25 m, identifying call amplitude rather than degradation as the factor limiting female response. However, as response decreased with increasing call degradation, signal quality remains a factor in female preference. Calls of the sister taxon B. intermedia were equally attractive to B. membracioides females as were conspecific calls, while the more distinct calls of B. serrata were less preferred than those of both B. membracioides and B. intermedia. This indicates a lack of discriminatory ability against a similar sounding heterospecific.

Baker, MC & DE Gammon (2006) Persistence and change of vocal signals in natural populations of chickadees: annual sampling of the gargle call over eight seasons. Behaviour 143: 1473-1509.

A study of the gargle call, an agonistic call of the black-capped chickadee Poecile atricapillus, for eight successive fall/winter seasons in three resident populations in Colorado (USA) revealed the following: (1) Individuals had repertoires of calls averaging 8.1 distinct types per bird (range 2-18). (2) Among individuals in a local population many calls shared some of the component syllables constituting the whole gargle calls; often the shared syllables occurred in different locations in the sequences of syllables (syntax) making up the calls. (3) Individuals also shared many of the same calls (identical or near identical syllable sequences); thus in a local sample the ensemble of call types in the population (the population repertoire) could be found by recording less than 10 birds. (4) In each population, the ensemble of gargle call types found in a given season consisted of a core group of about 10 kinds of gargle calls that persisted over all or nearly all the eight seasons of the study, but also the ensemble included a number of call types that were present for only one or a few seasons, or that occurred intermittently across time. (5) The great majority of call types that were lost from a population were explained by the loss of individual birds (carriers) by emigration or death. (6) The great majority of call types new in a population in any given season were explained by the occurrence of a new bird in the sample (local recruitment or immigration). (7) Both qualitative classification of gargle syllables and whole gargle calls by the method of visual inspection of sound spectrograms and automated quantitative analysis of acoustic features of syllables and calls were carried out in some examples, and these comparisons gave highly concordant results. Long-term studies of cultural evolution of non-song calls are infrequent but important because the sources and strength of social selection on non-song vocalizations are often likely to differ from those social factors influencing cultural evolution in territorial songs, the more often studied kind of bird acoustic signals. As a primary finding of this study we showed, using fine-grained measurements of acoustic variables, that a population retains a set of core gargle groups, each group representing a cultural tradition (behavioral lineage) exhibiting virtually no drift in detailed structure across eight consecutive seasons. In a small set of samples of core groups going back to 1988, there also was no evidence of change within a vocal tradition. These results suggest the hypothesis of strong canalizing social selection on the acoustic structure of gargles.

Poesel, A & T Dabelsteen (2006) Three vocalization types in the blue tit Cyanistes caeruleus: a test of the different signal-value hypothesis. Behaviour 143: 1529-1545.

Many songbird species use a repertoire of different song types in close-range interactions. In male blue tits, Cyanistes caeruleus, this includes songs with and without trill as well as scolding-like vocalizations. We here investigate using playback experiments whether these vocalization types are used at different stages during aggressive interactions, which may be reflected in differential responses of receivers. We predict that territory owning receivers will vary their strength of response with the vocalization type of a simulated intruder and that an aggressive territorial response may include song type matching, i.e. the use of the same vocalization type as the intruder. Supporting the different-signal value hypothesis, scolding-like playback elicited a stronger approach response of focal males than playback of songs with or without trill. There was no difference in song rate nor do songs without trill elicit a different strength of response than songs with trill. Males did not differ significantly in the amount of song type matching between the treatments but males that landed on the loudspeaker with playback of either songs without trill or scolding-like vocalizations did not match playback songs at all. This suggests a trade-off between approach and song type matching where song type matching is superfluous during very close approaches. Our results may suggest that in the blue tit threat display may be graded from songs with or without trill to scolding-like vocalizations and eventual attack of an intruder.

Nuechterlein, GL & D Buitron (2006) Advertising by western grebes: bout-length variability and signal confusion in a multiple-use call. Behaviour 143: 1547-1562.

Studies of song variability in birds have contributed greatly to our understanding of how animals use vocal signals to communicate information concerning their individual identity, species, sex, and territorial or pairing status to other members of the population. Most of these studies have focused on the songs of male passerine songbirds, where learning plays an important role in the development of song variability. Here we examine variability within a multi-functional call of a nonpasserine species, the western grebe (Aechmophorus occidentalis). In this observational study, we focused on variability in the lengths of consecutive call-bouts given by individuals within different behavioral contexts. Early in the breeding season, Advertising appeared to serve as an initial contact between two lone courting birds, which then approached and eventually engaged one another in visual displays. After pairing, however, both sexes continued to use Advertising calls in a variety of contexts that involved mate recognition or parent-young communication. Cases of signal confusion or misidentification of pairing status were rare and generally brief, even within the contexts of a large colony. During the pre-nesting period, unpaired birds usually gave bouts of 3-5 calls, while paired birds gave 1- or 2-call bouts. During incubation, birds returning to their nests often Advertised repeatedly to their mate using 1- or 2-call bouts in nearly equal proportions. Replies by their mate from the nest were more likely to be 2- or 3-call bouts (74%) and appeared to guide the returning bird to the nest. Finally, post-nesting parents calling to their young or mates primarily used 1- or 2-call bouts. We suggest that bout-length flexibility likely plays an important role in providing redundancy in a noisy environment and in limiting signal confusion in this multi-functional call.

<Prev in Thread] Current Thread [Next in Thread>
Admin

The University of NSW School of Computer and Engineering takes no responsibility for the contents of this archive. It is purely a compilation of material sent by many people to the Bioacoustics-L mailing list. It has not been checked for accuracy nor its content verified in any way. If you wish to get material removed from the archive or have other queries about the archive e-mail Andrew Taylor at this address: andrewt@cse.unsw.EDU.AU